• View in gallery
    Figure 1—

    Plot of the number of dogs evaluated for behavioral problems at the ABC from 1991 to 2001. The y-axis is scaled as the square root of the number of dogs. The straight line represents the linear regression line for the data (slope = 0.214; SE = 0.05;r2 = 0.67; P = 0.002).

  • View in gallery
    Figure 2—

    Distribution of ages (%, compared with overall population) among dogs evaluated for behavioral problems at the ABC and all dogs evaluated at CUHA (reference population).

  • 1

    Guy NC, Luescher UA, Dohoo IR, et al. Demographic and aggressive characteristics of dogs in a general veterinary caseload. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2001;74:1528.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 2

    Woodbury D. Fido's no longer in the doghouse. Trends Magazine 1998;14:4243.

  • 3

    Woodbury D. Risking life or limb for Fido. Trends Magazine 1999;15:3033.

  • 4

    Marder A, Rogers M, Engel J. Prevalence of canine behaviors in homes, in Proceedings. Annu Symp Anim Behav Res 2004;6263.

  • 5

    American Animal Hospital Association. AAHA pet owner survey results. Trends Magazine 1993;9:3233.

  • 6

    American Pet Products Manufacturers Association. 1999–2000 APPMA national pet owners survey. Greenwich, Conn: American Pet Products Manufacturers Association Inc, 1999;26, 94.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 7

    American Pet Products Manufacturers Association. 2003–2004 APPMA national pet owners survey. Greenwich, Conn: American Pet Products Manufacturers Association Inc, 2003;54, 104.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 8

    Adams GJ, Clark WT. The prevalence of behavioural problems in domestic dogs; a survey of 105 dog owners. Aust Vet Pract 1989;19:135137.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 9

    Mertens PA, Dodman NH. The diagnosis of behavioral problems in dogs, cats, horses and birds—characteristics of 323 cases (July 1994–June 1995). Part 1: dogs. Kleintierpraxis 1996;41:197206.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 10

    Appleby D, Magnus E, Bailey Q, et al. Data from the APBC annual review of cases, 1994–2003. Available at: www.apbc.org.uk/data.htm. Accessed May 1, 2005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 11

    Borchelt PL. Aggressive behavior of dogs kept as companion animals: classification and influence of sex, reproductive status and breed. Appl Anim Ethol 1983;10:4561.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 12

    Blackshaw JK. Abnormal behaviour in dogs. Aust Vet J 1988;65:393394.

  • 13

    Denenberg S, Landsberg GM, Horwitz D, et al. A comparison of cases referred to behaviorists in three different countries, in Proceedings. In: Current issues and research in veterinary behavioral medicine: papers presented at 5th International Veterinary Behavorial Meeting. West LaFayette, Ind: Purdue University Press, 2005;5662.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 14

    Fatjo J, Amat M, Ruiz de La Torre JL, et al. Small animal behavior problems in small animal practice in Spain, in Proceedings. Annu Symp Anim Behav Res 2002;5556.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 15

    Knol BW. Behavioural problems in dogs. Vet Q 1987;9:226234.

  • 16

    Hart BL, Hart LA. Canine and feline behavioral therapy. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger, 1985;2325.

  • 17

    Landsberg GM. The distribution of canine behavior cases at three behavior referral practices. Vet Med 1991;86:10111018.

  • 18

    Wright JC, Nesselrote MS. Classification of behavior problems in dogs: distributions of age, breed, sex and reproductive status. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1987;19:169178.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 19

    Voith VL. Clinical animal behavior. Calif Vet 1979;33:2125.

  • 20

    Voith VL. Clinical animal behavior: profile of 100 animal behavior cases. Mod Vet Pract 1981;62:483484.

  • 21

    Lund JD, Agger JF, Vestergaard KS. Reported behaviour problems in pet dogs in Denmark: age distribution and influence of breed and gender. Prev Vet Med 1996;28:3348.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 22

    Analytical Software. Statistix 7: user's manual. 7th ed. Tallahassee, Fla: Analytical Software, 2000;130131, 154, 163165, 192193, 255257.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 23

    Martin P, Bateson P. Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1993;145147, 150.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 24

    Cameron AC, Trivedi PK. Regression analysis of count data. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998;8890.

  • 25

    Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. Applied logistic regression. 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc, 2000;1617, 39.

  • 26

    Horowitz LM. Elements of statistics for psychology and education. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co, 1974;50, 365, 370.

  • 27

    Beaver BV. Clinical classification of canine aggression. Appl Anim Ethol 1983;10:3543.

  • 28

    Beaver BV. Profiles of dogs presented for aggression. J Am Anim Hosp Assoc 1993;29:564569.

  • 29

    Blackshaw JK. An overview of types of aggressive behaviour in dogs and methods of treatment. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1991;30:351361.

  • 30

    Fatjo J, Amat M, Mariotti V, et al. Aggression in dogs: analysis of 761 cases, in Proceedings. Current issues and research in veterinary behavioral medicine: papers presented at 5th International Veterinary Behavorial Meeting. West LaFayette, Ind: Purdue University Press, 2005;251254.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 31

    Guy NC, Luescher UA, Dohoo SE, et al. A case series of biting dogs: characteristics of the dogs, their behaviour and their victims. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2001;74:4357.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 32

    Simpson BS. Treatment of separation-related anxiety in dogs with clomipramine: results from a multicenter, blinded, placebo controlled clinical trial, in Proceedings. 1st Int Conf Vet Behav Med 1997;143154.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 33

    Beaver BV. Canine aggression. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1993;37:8182.

  • 34

    Houpt KA. Disruption of the human-companion animal bond: aggressive behavior in dogs. In: Katcher AH, Beck AM, eds. New perspectives on our lives with companion animals. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvannia Press, 1983;197204.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 35

    Takeuchi Y, Ogata N, Houpt KA, et al. Differences in background and outcome of three behavior problems in dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2001;70:297308.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 36

    Flannigan G, Dodman NH. Risk factors and behaviors associated with separation anxiety in dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2001;219:460466.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 37

    Galac S, Knol BW. Fear-motivated aggression in dogs: patient characteristics, diagnosis and therapy. Anim Welf 1997;6:915.

  • 38

    Line S, Voith VL. Dominance aggression of dogs towards people: behavior profile and response to treatment. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1986;16:7783.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 39

    Wright JC. Canine aggression toward people. In: Marder AR, Voith VL, eds. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract 1991;21:299314.

  • 40

    Sherman CK, Reisner IR, Taliaferro LA, et al. Characteristics, treatment, and outcome of 99 cases of aggression between dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1996;47:91108.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 41

    Overall KL, Dunham AE, Frank D. Frequency of nonspecific clinical signs in dogs with separation anxiety, thunderstorm phobia, and noise phobia, alone or in combination. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2001;219:467473.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 42

    Reisner IR. Behavioral drugs in the treatment of canine aggression, in Proceedings. AVMA Conv 2003;14.

  • 43

    Reisner I. Differential diagnosis and management of human-directed aggression in dogs. In: Houpt KA, Virga V, eds. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract 2003;33:303320.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 44

    Frank D. Interdog aggression: are we dealing with normal behaviors or anxiety-related behaviors?, in Proceedings. 140th Annu Conv Am Vet Med Assoc 2003;13.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Advertisement

Signalment factors, comorbidity, and trends in behavior diagnoses in dogs: 1,644 cases (1991–2001)

Michelle BambergerDepartment of Clinical Sciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853.

Search for other papers by Michelle Bamberger in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
 MS, DVM
and
Katherine A. HouptDepartment of Clinical Sciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853.

Search for other papers by Katherine A. Houpt in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
 VMD, PhD, DACVB
View More View Less

Abstract

Objective—To determine trends in behavior diagnoses; assess the relationship between diagnoses and age, sex, reproductive status, and breed; and evaluate associations between diagnoses within the same dog (comorbidity).

Design—Retrospective case series.

Animals—1,644 dogs.

Procedures—Medical records of dogs evaluated for behavioral problems were reviewed for breed, sex, reproductive status, consultation year, birth date, and diagnoses.

Results—Numbers of dogs with aggression, anxiety, and unruly behavior increased over the course of the study, as did the total number of dogs evaluated for behavioral problems. In general and for aggression, Dalmatians, English Springer Spaniels, German Shepherd Dogs, and mixed-breed dogs were evaluated more often than expected, whereas Labrador Retrievers and Golden Retrievers were evaluated less often than expected. Labrador Retrievers were also underrepresented for anxiety, whereas mixed-breed dogs were overrepresented. Males were overrepresented except for interdog aggression, anxieties, and phobias, whereas females were overrepresented for phobias. Dogs with phobias were evaluated at a median age of 6.5 years, compared with dogs with other problems (median age, 2.5 years). A mean of 1.6 diagnoses/dog was observed, with certain diagnoses clustered.

Conclusions and Clinical Relevance—Results suggested that in dogs, behavioral problems changed over the course of the study; age, sex, and breed distributions varied among diagnoses; and certain diagnoses were likely to occur together.

Abstract

Objective—To determine trends in behavior diagnoses; assess the relationship between diagnoses and age, sex, reproductive status, and breed; and evaluate associations between diagnoses within the same dog (comorbidity).

Design—Retrospective case series.

Animals—1,644 dogs.

Procedures—Medical records of dogs evaluated for behavioral problems were reviewed for breed, sex, reproductive status, consultation year, birth date, and diagnoses.

Results—Numbers of dogs with aggression, anxiety, and unruly behavior increased over the course of the study, as did the total number of dogs evaluated for behavioral problems. In general and for aggression, Dalmatians, English Springer Spaniels, German Shepherd Dogs, and mixed-breed dogs were evaluated more often than expected, whereas Labrador Retrievers and Golden Retrievers were evaluated less often than expected. Labrador Retrievers were also underrepresented for anxiety, whereas mixed-breed dogs were overrepresented. Males were overrepresented except for interdog aggression, anxieties, and phobias, whereas females were overrepresented for phobias. Dogs with phobias were evaluated at a median age of 6.5 years, compared with dogs with other problems (median age, 2.5 years). A mean of 1.6 diagnoses/dog was observed, with certain diagnoses clustered.

Conclusions and Clinical Relevance—Results suggested that in dogs, behavioral problems changed over the course of the study; age, sex, and breed distributions varied among diagnoses; and certain diagnoses were likely to occur together.

Owners' perceptions of behavioral problems in dogs as well as practitioners' interest in managing behavioral problems may lead to changes over time in the numbers and types of cases evaluated by general practitioners and referred to major university centers. An understanding by general practitioners of current trends as well as age, sex, and breed distributions of such dogs may aid them in making the correct diagnosis. Knowledge of comorbidity may also help the practitioner focus questions during evaluation of the dog's history, resulting in a more efficient consultation. With such knowledge, general practitioners can educate their clientele, who may then be more likely to report behavioral problems and seek resolution. Also, if educators understand current behavioral trends and case demographics, clinical training of veterinary students as well as continuing education may be benefited.

Behavioral problems have been evaluated via owner surveys1–8,a,b as well as case-review studies.9–20 Several studies4,8,a based on owners' opinions have revealed unruly behavior (barking and jumping up) as the most common problem confronting dog owners, whereas others have revealed house soiling2,3,5,b or nonbehavioral concerns (sadness when the pet dies, finding care when away, and shedding)6,7 as the primary problem. In contrast, results of most canine case studies9–15,17,18,20 indicate that aggression is the most prevalent behavioral diagnosis in dogs. Reports of several studies describe age, sex, and breed distributions in dogs with behavioral problems1,4,6,7,9-14,17-21 and associations among diagnoses.11–18

Most behavioral studies involve analysis of data during a specified period and do not evaluate trends over time, although a few have evaluated monthly and seasonal trends in overall behavioral problems10,b or have provided yearly overview data2,3,5-7,10 with some analysis from year to year.7,10 The primary objective of the study reported here was to determine trends in behavior diagnoses made at the ABC from 1991 to 2001. Secondary objectives included assessing the relationship between behavior diagnoses and signalment factors (age, sex, reproductive status, and breed), assessing the distribution of these factors over time, and evaluating comorbidity.

Criteria for Selection of Cases

Medical records for 1,668 dogs evaluated at the ABC from January 1, 1991, through December 31, 2001, were evaluated for this study; 24 dogs were excluded from the population because of incomplete data; therefore, 1,644 dogs were included in the study.

Procedures

Data on breed, sex, reproductive status, consultation year, birth date, and behavior diagnoses were gathered on each dog. A maximum of 3 diagnoses were taken for each dog; these diagnoses were the first 3 listed in the record. Dogs (n = 57,196) evaluated at the CUHA over the same period served as the reference population for breed, sex, reproductive status, and age comparisons. Breed, sex, reproductive status, and age data from the reference population were gathered independently. Over the entire study, a breed was assigned to all dogs and sex and reproductive status were specified for > 98% of the dogs. The owners were able to specify the age in 75% of the dogs. Ninety-six individual diagnoses taken from original records were assigned to the following general categories: aggression, anxieties, locomotor behaviors, ingestive behaviors, selfdirected aggression, grooming behaviors, fears, house soiling, phobias, sexual behaviors, unruly behaviors, vocalization behaviors, and miscellaneous behaviors (cognitive dysfunction, depression, pseudocyesis, psychogenic salivation, and hyperesthesia syndrome; Appendix). Individual diagnoses listed under the general categories of aggression, fears, and house soiling were divided into several subcategories. In the aggression category, subcategories were defined by target; defined as people (owners or strangers), animals, and things; and then further defined into individual diagnoses by motivation or etiology. Fears were grouped into subcategories by triggers as fear of people, animals, or situations. House soiling diagnoses were placed into either marking or elimination subcategories.

Statistical analysis—Diagnoses were analyzed on the levels category, subcategory, and individual. For any given level of diagnosis, no dog was counted more than once. Each diagnosis was initially evaluated by determining the number of dogs with that diagnosis over the 11-year study period, compared with the total number of dogs with any diagnosis, and expressing this as a percentage value. Each diagnosis assigned to an absolute number of 32 or more dogs over the entire study was then analyzed for trends over time and, secondarily, for the relationships between diagnoses and age, sex, reproductive status, and breed. Diagnoses assigned to < 32 dogs over the entire study typically had 2 or fewer dogs in most years, making analysis of trends over time difficult or impossible. Where an individual diagnosis was assigned to ≥ 95% of the dogs in a particular subcategory (eg, barking comprised 97.7% of the vocalization cases), only the subcategory was reported and discussed because the results (relationship with age, sex, reproductive status, and breed) were the same. To detect trends over time for all diagnoses, a least squares linear regression was performed by use of the square root of the number of cases (ie, the No. of dogs with a specific diagnosis) within a given year as the dependent variable and the year of diagnosis as the independent variable.22,23 A trend was defined as a slope that was significantly (P < 0.05) different from 0. An upward trend in a diagnosis was defined as an increasing annual percentage of total cases over time (positive slope), and a downward trend in a diagnosis was defined as a decreasing annual percentage of total cases (negative slope). A square root transformation was used to normalize errors in the data set.23,24 To detect trends over time for each diagnosis, logistic regression was performed by use of a proportion (the No. of cases of that particular diagnosis in a given year divided by the total No. of cases in that year) as the dependent variable and the year of diagnosis as the independent variable. Because the dependent variable was a proportion and not a direct count, logistic regression was used instead of linear regression.22,25 Neither regression was weighted because no individual value for a dependent variable was more important than any other.26

To determine the relationship between the age of dogs and each diagnosis, descriptive statistics (median and interquartile range) were first calculated for all dogs with each diagnosis over the 11-year study period as well as for all remaining dogs (dogs that did not have that diagnosis) over the same period. Median and interquartile range were used because the histogram of the number of cases (1-year bins) versus age did not follow a Gaussian distribution.26 To determine whether a significant difference between the ages of these 2 groups (dogs with a diagnosis and those without) existed, the log of the age was compared by use of a 2sample t test22 to more closely approximate a Gaussian distribution. To determine clinical importance, a difference of > 2 years was set to account for dogs in which age was estimated. These same methods were also used over all diagnoses for use of breed (mixed breed vs purebred), sex, and reproductive status as grouping variables. To assess the relationship between age of dogs over all diagnoses from year to year, the median age was determined for each year and a least squares linear regression was performed with the median age for each year as the dependent variable and the year of diagnosis as the independent variable.

To assess the relationship between the caseload (the total No. of dogs evaluated) and sex of dogs over the study period, the percentage of total dogs for total (intact and neutered), sexually intact, and neutered males as well as total, sexually intact, and spayed females was determined. The 2-sample proportion test was used to compare percentages between males and females and between sexually intact and neutered dogs within the ABC population; this test is used to compare proportions in 2 independent samples.22 Between the ABC and CUHA populations, the 1-sample proportion test was used to compare total males with total males, castrated males with castrated males, and spayed females with spayed females; the corresponding proportion in the CUHA population was treated as fixed and was used to define the null hypothesis for this test. The 1-sample proportion test is used to test whether a proportion differs from a hypothesized value.22 To assess the relationship between the caseload and sex of dogs from year to year, the percentage of total dogs for total males, neutered males, and spayed females was determined for each year and analyzed by use of logistic regression; regressions between the ABC and the CUHA populations were compared by use of a Wald test that treated the CUHA population as fixed. Specifically, a test statistic for the equality of slopes was computed by taking the absolute value of the difference of the slopes from the 2 regressions divided by the SE of the slope in the ABC population.25 A 2-tailed P value was then computed by use of a normal distribution.

To assess the relationship between the caseload and breed of dog during the total study period, the percentage of total dogs of each breed (total No. of dogs of each breed divided by all dogs of all breeds) during the study was determined. Breeds with ≥ 30 dogs during the study were compared with the percentage of total dogs of each breed of CUHA dogs during the same time by use of a 1-sample proportion test. The relationship between each diagnosis and breed of dog was analyzed in the same manner; the percentage of total dogs of the 4 top breeds (including mixed breed) in each diagnosis was compared with the percentage of total dogs of the same breeds evaluated by the CUHA over the same time by use of a 1-sample proportion test. To assess the relationship between the caseload and breed of dog from year to year, the percentage of total dogs for breeds with 30 or more dogs over the study was analyzed via logistic regression; regressions between the ABC and CUHA populations were compared in the same manner as described previously.

The level of association between 2 diagnoses occurring within the same dog was assessed by first determining the probability of each diagnosis given that the other diagnosis was present. The significance of this association was then evaluated by use of the Pearson χ2 test.22,26

All analyses were performed with standard software.c All tests were 2-tailed, and values of P < 0.05 were considered significant.

Results

The number of dogs evaluated at the ABC increased significantly (P = 0.002) between 1991 and 2001 (Figure 1). This trend was also seen in the CUHA population over the same period (slope of the regression line = 1.114; SE = 0.14; r2 = 0.87; P < 0.001); a significant (P < 0.001) difference was found between the ABC and CUHA populations when regressions were compared—the CUHA population increased at a greater rate than the ABC population.

Figure 1—
Figure 1—

Plot of the number of dogs evaluated for behavioral problems at the ABC from 1991 to 2001. The y-axis is scaled as the square root of the number of dogs. The straight line represents the linear regression line for the data (slope = 0.214; SE = 0.05;r2 = 0.67; P = 0.002).

Citation: Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 229, 10; 10.2460/javma.229.10.1591

Distribution of diagnoses—The number of dogs affected by each diagnosis (and percentages, compared with the total No. of affected dogs) for all major category diagnoses and all diagnoses that affected ≥ 32 dogs/y over the study period were determined (Table 1). Because each dog may have had up to 3 diagnoses, the sum of the individual percentages was > 100%. The category of aggression accounted for the largest percentage of affected dogs during the study period, followed by anxieties, unruly behavior, house soiling, phobias, excessive vocalization behavior, abnormal ingestive behavior, abnormal locomotor behavior, miscellaneous, fears, self-directed aggression, grooming behavior, and sexual behavior.

Table 1—

Distribution (number of affected dogs [%]) of diagnoses among 1,644 dogs evaluated for behavioral problems at the ABC from 1991 to 2001.

DiagnosisNo. of dogs (%)
Aggression1,191 (72.4)
   People-directed aggression997 (60.6)
      Owner-directed aggression724 (44.0)
         Dominance-related aggression667 (40.6)
         Fear aggression directed at owners85 (5.2)
      Stranger-directed aggression535 (32.5)
         Fear aggression directed at strangers276 (16.8)
         Territorial aggression339 (20.6)
   Animal-directed aggression304 (18.5)
      Interdog aggression268 (16.3)
Anxieties324 (19.7)
   General anxiety93 (5.7)
   Separation anxiety236 (14.4)
Locomotor behavior16 (1.0)
Ingestive behavior23 (1.4)
Self-directed aggression8 (0.5)
Grooming behavior9 (0.5)
Fears11 (0.7)
House soiling124 (7.5)
   Elimination117 (7.1)
   Urination and defecation99 (6.0)
Miscellaneous14 (0.9)
Phobias64 (3.9)
   Storm phobia37 (2.3)
Sexual behavior2 (0.1)
Unruly behavior201 (12.2)
   Attention-seeking behavior126 (7.7)
   Destructive behavior40 (2.4)
Vocalization behavior45 (2.7)
   Barking44 (2.7)

Percentages do not add to 100% because each dog may have had up to 3 diagnoses. Major category diagnoses and all other diagnoses with > 32 cases/y over the study period are listed.

Trends in diagnoses—Trends were detected for several diagnoses (Table 2). In the category of aggresion, upward trends were seen in fear aggression directed at owners, stranger-directed aggression, and fear aggression directed at strangers. A downward trend was found in dominance-related aggression. Upward trends were also detected in anxieties as well as in general and separation anxiety. Downward trends were observed in house soiling, elimination, and urination and defecation. Upward trends were observed in unruly and attention-seeking behaviors, whereas a downward trend was observed in destructive behavior.

Table 2—

Results of logistic regression analysis of the frequency of various behavior diagnoses in dogs from 1991 to 2001.

DiagnosisSlopeSEP value
Dominance-related aggression−0.0380.0160.019
Fear aggression directed at owners0.1420.039< 0.001
Stranger-directed aggression0.1220.018< 0.001
Fear aggression directed at strangers0.1990.024< 0.001
Anxieties0.1410.021< 0.001
   General anxiety0.1710.038< 0.001
   Separation anxiety0.1070.024< 0.001
House soiling−0.0970.0310.002
   Elimination−0.1040.0310.001
   Urination and defecation−0.1450.035< 0.001
Unruly behavior0.0690.0250.006
   Attention-seeking behavior0.2630.037< 0.001
   Destructive behavior−0.1350.0530.011

Slope = Slope of the regression line. P value indicates comparison with a slope of 0.

Relationship between diagnoses and age—The distribution of age for the CUHA population differed from that of the ABC population (Figure 2). Because of differences between the 2 populations, the relationship between age and diagnosis was determined by making comparisons within the ABC population (dogs with the diagnosis vs dogs without the diagnosis). Overall, median age at evaluation was 2.5 years, mean age was 3.7 years, and interquartile range was 1.5 to 5.5 years. Because exact age was estimated to the nearest month or year in some dogs, only diagnoses in dogs with an age difference (between dogs with the diagnosis and those without) of > 2 years were considered to have clinical importance. Only phobias (median age, 6.5 years; interquartile range, 4.8 to 9.5 years) and storm phobia (median age, 6.5 years; interquartile range, 5.5 to 9 years) were significantly (P < 0.001) different between dogs with the diagnosis and dogs without the diagnosis (median age, 2.5 years; interquartile range, 1.5 to 4.5 years) and considered clinically important. No difference was detected in median ages from year to year; corresponding data from the CUHA also yielded no difference in median ages.

Figure 2—
Figure 2—

Distribution of ages (%, compared with overall population) among dogs evaluated for behavioral problems at the ABC and all dogs evaluated at CUHA (reference population).

Citation: Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 229, 10; 10.2460/javma.229.10.1591

Relationship between diagnoses and sex—Sex differences among dogs with various diagnoses were determined (Table 3). Overall, more total male dogs and more neutered dogs were seen in the ABC population, compared with the CUHA population. Sex differences were observed in the categories of aggression, anxieties, and phobias, with more total males in most of the aggression diagnoses categories but more total females in the interdog aggression, anxieties, and phobias categories. General anxiety was the only diagnosis for which the percentage of neutered dogs (males) was not higher than the corresponding percentage in the CUHA population. The only change in sex or reproductive status over time was an upward trend in spayed females (slope of the regression line, 0.044; SE = 0.017; P = 0.009). A similar trend was observed in the CUHA population (slope of the regression line, 0.036; SE = 0.003; P < 0.001). There was no significant difference when these regressions were compared (P = 0.646).

Table 3—

Distribution of sexes for dogs with various behavior diagnoses from 1991 to 2001.

DiagnosisMale (%)Female (%)
All diagnoses56.6*43.4
Aggression60.1*39.9
   People-directed aggression64.6*35.4
      Owner-directed aggression66.2*33.8
         Dominance-related aggression67.8*32.2
      Stranger-directed aggression64.7*35.3
         Fear aggression directed at strangers56.5*43.5
         Territorial aggression68.4*31.6
   Animal-directed aggression44.755.2
      Interdog aggression42.557.5
Anxieties45.054.9
   General anxiety24.775.3*
Phobias39.160.9

Significantly (P ≤ 0.05) greater than values in the opposite sex in the ABC population and values for the same sex in the corresponding reference CUHA population.

Significantly (P ≤ 0.05) greater than values in the opposite sex in the ABC population only. For the CUHA population, percentages of male and female dogs were 48.5% and 51.5%, respectively.

Relationship between diagnoses and breed—For breeds that had ≥ 30 dogs with any diagnosis and for breeds with the 4 highest percentages of dogs with each category of diagnosis, the percentages of affected dogs, compared with all affected dogs, were determined for the ABC population; corresponding percentages for these breeds were also determined for the CUHA population (Table 4). Across all diagnoses, affected Dalmatians, English Springer Spaniels, German Shepherd Dogs, and mixed-breed dogs constituted a significantly higher percentage of the total number of dogs in the ABC population, compared with the CUHA population; cases involving Golden Retrievers and Labrador Retrievers constituted a lower percentage of the total number of cases. With regard to breed distribution for all diagnoses over time, an upward trend was detected in mixed-breed dogs and a downward trend was detected in Cocker Spaniels, English Springer Spaniels, and German Shepherd Dogs (Table 5); these trends were also detected in the CUHA population. When regressions between the ABC and CUHA populations were compared, significant differences were detected in mixedbreed dogs (P = 0.012) and English Springer Spaniels (P < 0.001). In both breeds, the ABC population changed at a faster rate than did the CUHA population.

Table 4—

Distribution (%) of breeds in a reference (CUHA) population and among dogs with various behavior diagnoses evaluated at the ABC from 1991 to 2001.

BreedCUHAAll diagnosesAggressionAnxietyHouse soilingPhobiasUnruly behaviorVocalization
Beagle1.93.45.6*
Bichon Frise0.74.8*
Cairn Terrier0.68.9*
Cocker Spaniel3.82.96.5
Dachshund1.24.4
Dalmatian1.02.6*
Doberman Pinscher1.74.4
English Springer Spaniel1.74.1*5.2*
German Shepherd Dog4.85.8*6.5*4.06.34.54.4
Golden Retriever5.53.37.85.5
Jack Russell Terrier0.84.4*
Labrador Retriever9.54.94.55.64.78.54.4
Miniature Dachshund0.34.4*
Rottweiler2.42.2
Shetland Sheepdog1.58.9*
Mixed24.730.7*29.6*39.2*22.634.432.8*24.4

For the CUHA population,values indicate percentage distribution of dogs for each breed.

Significantly (P ≤ 0.05) greater than that of the corresponding CUHA population.

Significantly (P ≤ 0.05) less than that of the CUHA population. All diagnoses = breeds with 30 or more cases; for all other diagnoses, values are given for the top 4 breeds with regard to the number of dogs with each diagnosis, except for the category vocalization, for which the diagnosis was equally evident in > 4 breeds.

Table 5—

Results of logistic regression analysis of various breeds of dogs evaluated at the ABC (1991-2001) versus year, and analysis of breed versus year in a reference population of dogs at the CUHA.

PopulationSlopeSEP value
ABC
   Mixed breed0.0610.017< 0.001
   Cocker Spaniel−0.1070.0480.025
   English Springer Spaniel−0.0830.0410.04
   German Shepherd Dog−0.0710.0360.046
CUHA
   Mixed breed0.0190.003< 0.001
   Cocker Spaniel−0.0180.0070.007
   English Springer Spaniel−0.060.01< 0.001
   German Shepherd Dog−0.0310.006< 0.001

P value indicates comparison with a slope of 0.

Comorbidity—Of all dogs, 55.4% had 1 diagnosis, 26% had 2 diagnoses, and 18.6% had ≥ 3 diagnoses made at the time of evaluation. The mean number of diagnoses per dog was 1.6, and certain diagnoses occurred in clusters. Significant associations between 2 diagnoses were detected (Table 6). Most of the significant associations involved cases with both diagnoses from the aggression category.

Table 6—

Associations between pairs of diagnoses in 1,644 dogs evaluated at the ABC from 1991 to 2001.

Diagnosis 1Diagnosis 2No.D1D2P value
Fear aggression directed at strangersTerritorial aggression105276 (38.0)339 (30.9)< 0.001
Fear aggression directed at ownersFear aggression directed at strangers5685 (65.9)276 (20.3)< 0.001
Dominance-related aggressionFear aggression directed at owners52667 (7.8)85 (61.2)< 0.001
Dominance-related aggressionInterdog aggression47667 (7.0)268 (17.5)< 0.001
Interdog aggressionTerritorial aggression37268 (13.8)339 (10.9)0.003
Dominance-related aggressionSeparation anxiety21667 (3.1)236 (8.9)< 0.001
Fear aggression directed at strangersSeparation anxiety17276 (6.2)236 (7.2)< 0.001
Dominance-related aggressionGeneral anxiety15667 (2.2)93 (16.1)< 0.001
Territorial aggressionSeparation anxiety10339 (2.9)236 (4.2)< 0.001
Noise phobiaSeparation anxiety1027 (37.0)236 (4.2)< 0.001
General anxietyInterdog aggression693 (6.5)268 (2.2)0.008

No. = Number of dogs with both diagnoses. D1 = Number of dogs with diagnosis 1 (percentage of dogs that also had diagnosis 2 in parentheses). D2 = Number of dogs with diagnosis 2 (percentage of dogs that also had diagnosis 1 in parentheses). The last column gives the value of P as determined by the Pearson χ2 test.

Discussion

Aggression was diagnosed in nearly 75% of all dogs evaluated, mostly because of aggression directed at humans (mostly owners) rather than other animals. Dominance-related diagnoses accounted for the majority of owner-directed aggression (92%), whereas fear aggression directed at owners accounted for most of the remaining cases. Clearly, owner-directed aggression has been10,19,27-31,d and continues to be a difficult problem for veterinarians and owners to manage. Although not as prevalent as aggression, anxieties are often considered to be the next most frequently reported problem, with separation anxiety being the most commonly seen problem in this category.10,13

Upward trends were detected in several diagnoses in the categories of aggression (stranger-directed aggression, fear aggression directed at strangers, and fear aggression directed at owners) and anxieties (general and separation). It is imperative that veterinarians, in training and at the level of continuing education, be aware of current behavioral problems and understand how to advise clients in prevention and management. This is especially important in the case of strangerdirected aggression, for which upward trends may be a sign of the increasingly litigious nature of society. It is interesting that the percentage of dominance-related aggression cases decreased over time. This may be partially because of trends in current terminology. Few studies have tracked caseloads over extended periods of time. However, in a review of canine cases in 2000,10 the referral rate of status-related aggression had declined and fear aggression had increased since 1996; these results support our findings. Although Appleby et al10 reported a decline in the referral rate for separation anxiety from 1996 to 2000, an increase among dogs obtained from rescue was detected. In the study reported here, the upward trends detected in separation anxiety may have been attributable to greater awareness of this problem by veterinarians as well as pet owners and the emergence during this time period of an approved medication to treat separation anxiety.32

Breed incidence of behavioral problems varies depending on many factors including breed distribution, sample size, geographic location, time of study, reference population, and source of puppies. When a behavioral problem has been clearly identified within a breed, it is important that veterinarians, breeders, dog owners, and prospective owners be alerted that such problems exist. Overall, in our study, many more (30.7%) dogs of mixed breed (unknown crosses as well as known crosses) were evaluated than dogs of any pure breed, although purebred dogs as a group were more than twice as numerous as mixed-breed dogs. This was also true of the CUHA population that included 24.7% mixed-breed dogs and has been reported by others.13,17,20,31 Golden Retrievers and Labrador Retrievers were underrepresented in general and specifically for aggression problems, whereas Dalmatians, English Springer Spaniels, German Shepherd Dogs, and mixedbreed dogs were overrepresented in these areas. Some or all of the breeds in the latter group have also been reported more frequently by others for general problems11,17,27,33,34 as well as for aggression.1,11,13,17,27,30,33-35 In our study, Beagles, Dalmatians, and mixed-breed dogs had separation anxiety more often than expected and the number of mixed-breed dogs (n = 104) with separation anxiety far outnumbered the group of purebred dogs (17) with separation anxiety. However, breed distribution of separation anxiety varies among previous studies. Results implicate mixed-breed dogs35 and purebred dogse as being evaluated more frequently than expected or that there is no difference between these groups.36

Regarding the incidence of all diagnoses over the study period for various breeds, an upward trend was detected in mixed-breed dogs and downward trends in Cocker Spaniels, English Springer Spaniels, and German Shepherd Dogs. These trends were also seen in the CUHA population, but changes in incidences of all diagnoses in mixed breeds and English Springer Spaniels were significantly greater in the ABC population. It is important to note that all of these breeds except Cocker Spaniels were evaluated more often than would be expected for many diagnoses in the aggression category; perhaps the popularity of these breeds in our geographic location has decreased because of behavioral problems such as aggression.

In addition to breed distribution, veterinarians and owners need to know at what age certain problems are more likely to appear to prevent them altogether or to diagnose them in the early stages of development. Median age at evaluation of all dogs for behavior problems was 2.5 years, and mean age was 3.7 years. Others have reported similar findings17,18,21 or found that the largest age categories included these ages.9,10,13 In the study reported here, phobias and storm phobias occurred at a median age of 6.5 years, compared with a median age of 2.5 years for all other diagnoses. Landsberg17 did not analyze differences among ages in various diagnoses but reported that all diagnoses occurred at a mean of 2 to 3 years of age, except for phobias, which occurred at a mean of 5 years of age. The older age at evaluation implies that learning plays an important role in development of phobias in dogs, even more so than in other diagnoses, for which median ages ranged from 2.5 to 3.5 years. In assessing age distribution over time, median ages for a given year were remarkably static because all were 2.5 years except the year 1992, for which median age at evaluation was 3.5 years.

It is also necessary to understand whether sex plays a role in development of problem behaviors. In the study reported here, males were evaluated more often than expected overall and for many aggression problems (56.6% overall; 60.1% aggression); others have reported similar results.1,9-14,17-21,27,28,30,33-35,37-39,d Neutered dogs were evaluated more frequently than sexually intact dogs by more than a factor of 4, which was most likely because male and female dogs are often routinely castrated and spayed before puberty. For most behavior diagnoses, neutered dogs were also evaluated more often than expected, compared with the CUHA population. Although similar findings have been reported in recent studies,10,13,14 other studies covering periods before neutering is typically performed have detected a preponderance of sexually intact males20,37,d or sexually intact males and spayed females11,18,34 evaluated for aggression. It is important to note that animal-directed and interdog aggression (including household and nonhousehold aggression) were the only aggression diagnoses for which females were evaluated more often than expected in our study. However, reported sex differences in interdog aggression vary. Some authors have found that males are more likely to fight than females, especially with other males,11,33 whereas others have found no sex differences.17 Sherman et al,40 using data from 3 of the years (1991 to 1993) reported in our study, determined that females are more likely to initiate interdog household aggression, whereas males are more likely to attack nonhousehold dogs. We also found that females were evaluated more often than males for anxieties, general anxiety, and phobias. Lund et al21 reported that males had a significantly lower risk of general anxiety, and Appleby et al10 reported that phobic behavior was higher in females in the 1994 caseload analysis. However, Overall et al41 found that there was no sex difference in regard to the relative percentage of total cases of thunderstorm phobia, noise phobia, and separation anxiety (discrepancy may be attributable to higher sample size of the present study). Numbers of spayed females increased over the study in the ABC population as well as the CUHA population, with a significantly greater change seen in the ABC population; this difference was most likely attributable to more sexually intact females being evaluated at CUHA because of referrals for reproductive problems.

In the study reported here, certain diagnoses occurred together more often than chance would predict. The practitioner could use this knowledge during evaluation of the dog's history to help focus questions and uncover problems of which even the owner may have been unaware. The highest number of combinations of diagnoses occurred within the category of aggression, and the next most frequently occurring group of combinations was diagnoses in the aggression and anxiety categories. Owners with dogs with fear aggression toward strangers should be questioned specifically on issues of fear aggression toward family members because these diagnoses are often associated. Also, contexts and postures during aggressive incidents in the home should be clearly defined because we found an association with fear in dogs with dominance-related aggression. Dogs that fight with other dogs may also have issues in the home with owners (dominance-related) or with strangers (territorial). We found 6 pairs of aggression-anxiety diagnoses in at least 6 dogs over the study. This is not surprising because anxiety may lower a dog's threshold for aggression.42,43 Because of this, dogs with aggression (dominance-related, fear towards strangers, interdog, and territorial) should be observed carefully during the consultation for signs of anxiety and inability to relax,44 and owners should be questioned for signs of separation anxiety or general anxiety in their dogs. Noise phobia and separation anxiety also occurred together more often than chance would predict. Such results have been reported in 2 other studies.36,41 It is important to keep in mind that these data represent only those dogs referred to veterinary behaviorists and that breed distributions reported in this study do not necessarily reflect breed prevalence of behavioral problems.

ABBREVIATIONS

ABC

Animal Behavior Clinic at Cornell University

CUHA

Cornell University Hospital for Animals

a.

Vacalopoulos A, Anderson RK. Canine behavior problems reported by clients in a study of veterinary hospitals (abstr). Appl Anim Behav Sci 1993;37:84.

b.

Marder A. Behavior problems after adoption (abstr), in Proceedings. Annu Symp Anim Behav Res 2002;6.

c.

Statistix 7, version 7, Analytical Software, Tallahassee, Fla.

d.

Beaver BV. Profiles of aggressive dogs (abstr), in Proceedings. 56th Annu Meet Am Anim Hosp Assoc 1989;635.

e.

Voith VL, Goodloe L, Chapman B. Comparison of dogs for behavior problems by source of dog (abstr), in Proceedings. 130th Annu Conv Am Vet Med Assoc 1993;54.

References

  • 1

    Guy NC, Luescher UA, Dohoo IR, et al. Demographic and aggressive characteristics of dogs in a general veterinary caseload. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2001;74:1528.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 2

    Woodbury D. Fido's no longer in the doghouse. Trends Magazine 1998;14:4243.

  • 3

    Woodbury D. Risking life or limb for Fido. Trends Magazine 1999;15:3033.

  • 4

    Marder A, Rogers M, Engel J. Prevalence of canine behaviors in homes, in Proceedings. Annu Symp Anim Behav Res 2004;6263.

  • 5

    American Animal Hospital Association. AAHA pet owner survey results. Trends Magazine 1993;9:3233.

  • 6

    American Pet Products Manufacturers Association. 1999–2000 APPMA national pet owners survey. Greenwich, Conn: American Pet Products Manufacturers Association Inc, 1999;26, 94.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 7

    American Pet Products Manufacturers Association. 2003–2004 APPMA national pet owners survey. Greenwich, Conn: American Pet Products Manufacturers Association Inc, 2003;54, 104.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 8

    Adams GJ, Clark WT. The prevalence of behavioural problems in domestic dogs; a survey of 105 dog owners. Aust Vet Pract 1989;19:135137.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 9

    Mertens PA, Dodman NH. The diagnosis of behavioral problems in dogs, cats, horses and birds—characteristics of 323 cases (July 1994–June 1995). Part 1: dogs. Kleintierpraxis 1996;41:197206.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 10

    Appleby D, Magnus E, Bailey Q, et al. Data from the APBC annual review of cases, 1994–2003. Available at: www.apbc.org.uk/data.htm. Accessed May 1, 2005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 11

    Borchelt PL. Aggressive behavior of dogs kept as companion animals: classification and influence of sex, reproductive status and breed. Appl Anim Ethol 1983;10:4561.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 12

    Blackshaw JK. Abnormal behaviour in dogs. Aust Vet J 1988;65:393394.

  • 13

    Denenberg S, Landsberg GM, Horwitz D, et al. A comparison of cases referred to behaviorists in three different countries, in Proceedings. In: Current issues and research in veterinary behavioral medicine: papers presented at 5th International Veterinary Behavorial Meeting. West LaFayette, Ind: Purdue University Press, 2005;5662.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 14

    Fatjo J, Amat M, Ruiz de La Torre JL, et al. Small animal behavior problems in small animal practice in Spain, in Proceedings. Annu Symp Anim Behav Res 2002;5556.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 15

    Knol BW. Behavioural problems in dogs. Vet Q 1987;9:226234.

  • 16

    Hart BL, Hart LA. Canine and feline behavioral therapy. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger, 1985;2325.

  • 17

    Landsberg GM. The distribution of canine behavior cases at three behavior referral practices. Vet Med 1991;86:10111018.

  • 18

    Wright JC, Nesselrote MS. Classification of behavior problems in dogs: distributions of age, breed, sex and reproductive status. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1987;19:169178.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 19

    Voith VL. Clinical animal behavior. Calif Vet 1979;33:2125.

  • 20

    Voith VL. Clinical animal behavior: profile of 100 animal behavior cases. Mod Vet Pract 1981;62:483484.

  • 21

    Lund JD, Agger JF, Vestergaard KS. Reported behaviour problems in pet dogs in Denmark: age distribution and influence of breed and gender. Prev Vet Med 1996;28:3348.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 22

    Analytical Software. Statistix 7: user's manual. 7th ed. Tallahassee, Fla: Analytical Software, 2000;130131, 154, 163165, 192193, 255257.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 23

    Martin P, Bateson P. Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1993;145147, 150.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 24

    Cameron AC, Trivedi PK. Regression analysis of count data. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998;8890.

  • 25

    Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. Applied logistic regression. 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc, 2000;1617, 39.

  • 26

    Horowitz LM. Elements of statistics for psychology and education. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co, 1974;50, 365, 370.

  • 27

    Beaver BV. Clinical classification of canine aggression. Appl Anim Ethol 1983;10:3543.

  • 28

    Beaver BV. Profiles of dogs presented for aggression. J Am Anim Hosp Assoc 1993;29:564569.

  • 29

    Blackshaw JK. An overview of types of aggressive behaviour in dogs and methods of treatment. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1991;30:351361.

  • 30

    Fatjo J, Amat M, Mariotti V, et al. Aggression in dogs: analysis of 761 cases, in Proceedings. Current issues and research in veterinary behavioral medicine: papers presented at 5th International Veterinary Behavorial Meeting. West LaFayette, Ind: Purdue University Press, 2005;251254.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 31

    Guy NC, Luescher UA, Dohoo SE, et al. A case series of biting dogs: characteristics of the dogs, their behaviour and their victims. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2001;74:4357.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 32

    Simpson BS. Treatment of separation-related anxiety in dogs with clomipramine: results from a multicenter, blinded, placebo controlled clinical trial, in Proceedings. 1st Int Conf Vet Behav Med 1997;143154.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 33

    Beaver BV. Canine aggression. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1993;37:8182.

  • 34

    Houpt KA. Disruption of the human-companion animal bond: aggressive behavior in dogs. In: Katcher AH, Beck AM, eds. New perspectives on our lives with companion animals. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvannia Press, 1983;197204.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 35

    Takeuchi Y, Ogata N, Houpt KA, et al. Differences in background and outcome of three behavior problems in dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2001;70:297308.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 36

    Flannigan G, Dodman NH. Risk factors and behaviors associated with separation anxiety in dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2001;219:460466.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 37

    Galac S, Knol BW. Fear-motivated aggression in dogs: patient characteristics, diagnosis and therapy. Anim Welf 1997;6:915.

  • 38

    Line S, Voith VL. Dominance aggression of dogs towards people: behavior profile and response to treatment. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1986;16:7783.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 39

    Wright JC. Canine aggression toward people. In: Marder AR, Voith VL, eds. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract 1991;21:299314.

  • 40

    Sherman CK, Reisner IR, Taliaferro LA, et al. Characteristics, treatment, and outcome of 99 cases of aggression between dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1996;47:91108.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 41

    Overall KL, Dunham AE, Frank D. Frequency of nonspecific clinical signs in dogs with separation anxiety, thunderstorm phobia, and noise phobia, alone or in combination. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2001;219:467473.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 42

    Reisner IR. Behavioral drugs in the treatment of canine aggression, in Proceedings. AVMA Conv 2003;14.

  • 43

    Reisner I. Differential diagnosis and management of human-directed aggression in dogs. In: Houpt KA, Virga V, eds. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract 2003;33:303320.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 44

    Frank D. Interdog aggression: are we dealing with normal behaviors or anxiety-related behaviors?, in Proceedings. 140th Annu Conv Am Vet Med Assoc 2003;13.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Appendix 1

Definitions of behavioral diagnoses for cats and dogs.

Aggression—a general term that includes all features of defense, threat, and attack behavior directed at people, animals, or things and excludes all features of affiliative behavior.
People-directed
Owner-directed
Dominance-related aggression—formerly known as dominance aggression, also known as conflict-related, impulse/control, and status-related aggression; includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, blocking, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at owners or family members over control of resources such as space, food, possessions, proximity to owner, or situations.
Aggression to children—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, blocking, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at familiar children.
Fear—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at owners and accompanied by attempts to escape; fearful postures such as tucked tail, crouched body, or supine with abdomen exposed; and flattened or pinned ears. This aggression may occur in any location, either on or off the owner's property or in both locations.
Herding—nipping, barking, and biting at the feet and ankles in an attempt to move owners in 1 direction.
Idiopathic—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at owners for which no medical or behavioral stimulus or cause has been discovered.
Irritable—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at owners in response to a broad range of situations, such as stress, frustration, or pain.
Noise-triggered aggression—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, or biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at owners that is solely initiated by a sound.
Pain-based—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) that may be triggered by an action, either internal or external to the pet, that the pet perceives as painful and that is directed at the owner.
Petting-induced aggression—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at a person after a certain number of pets to the animal's head or body.
Play—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at the owner or family members during a play session.
Predatory—stalking and hunting accompanied by attempts to kill or killing directed at owners, usually infants.
Redirected—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting directed at the owner when a pet is prevented from pursuing aggressive behavior directed at another target.
Stranger-directed
Aggression to children—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at unfamiliar children.
Fear—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at strangers and accompanied by attempts to escape and fearful postures such as tucked tail, crouched body or supine with abdomen exposed, and flattened or pinned ears. This aggression may occur in any location, either on or off the owner's property, or in both locations.
Idiopathic—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at strangers, for which no medical or behavioral stimulus or cause has been discovered.
Irritable—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at strangers in response to a broad range of situations, such as stress, frustration, or pain.
Pain-based—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) that may be triggered by an action, either internal or external to the pet, that the pet perceives as painful and that is directed at a stranger.
Petting-induced aggression—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at a stranger after a certain number of pets to the animal's head or body.
Play—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at strangers during a play session.
Predatory—stalking and hunting accompanied by attempts to kill or killing directed at strangers, usually infants. Redirected: includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at a stranger when a pet is prevented from pursuing aggressive behavior directed at another target.
Territorial—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) directed at strangers on the owner's property or what the animal considers to be the property.
Animal-directed
Fear—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at animals and accompanied by attempts to escape and fearful postures such as tucked tail, crouched body or supine with abdomen exposed, and flattened or pinned ears. This aggression may occur in any location, either on or off the owner's property, or in both locations.
Intercat—includes 3 or more of the following signs: staring, blocking, hissing, vocalizing, stalking, swatting, lunging, and biting among 2 or more cats within a household or among cats from different households.
Interdog—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting among 2 or more dogs within a household or among dogs from different households.
Interspecies—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at another species (exclusive of people) within the household or outside of the household, exclusive of predation.
Play—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, or biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at other animals, exclusive of people during a play session.
Predatory—stalking and hunting accompanied by attempts to kill or killing other animal species, exclusive of people.
Redirected—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting (dogs) or hissing, growling, and biting (cats) directed at another animal when a pet is prevented from pursuing another aggressive behavior.
Thing-directed
Aggression to cars—includes 2 or more of the following signs: growling, barking aggressively, snapping, snarling, lunging, and biting directed at parked or moving cars.
Anxieties—conditions resulting from the anticipation by the animal of danger or harm.
Anxiety-related pruritis—itching occurring during situations of stress, frustration, or conflict and having no medical cause.
Barrier frustration—inability to stay or be enclosed or confined in any way without extreme anxiety.
General anxiety—increased vigilance, motor activity, and autonomic activity that interfere with normal functioning.
Hyperattachment—remaining within sight or touch of the owner at all times.
Separation anxiety—includes 1 or more of the following signs: destructive behavior, vocalization, salivation, and elimination caused by physical or visual separation from the owner.
Travel anxiety—increased vigilance, motor activity, and autonomic activity during any type of travel.
Locomotor behavior—out of context, repetitive body movements that are difficult or impossible to interrupt and interfere with the animal's normal routine.
Circling—repetitive walking or running in a circular pattern for no apparent reason.
Light chasing—following light from any source in attempt to catch it, such that normal function is interfered with.
Shadow chasing—following shadows produced by any source in attempt to catch them, such that normal function is interfered with.
Tail chasing—repetitive and excessive chasing of the tail that may or may not result in alopecia, abrasions, or ulcerations.
Ingestive behavior—out of context, repetitive oral movements that are difficult or impossible to interrupt and interfere with the animal's normal routine and abnormal appetite or abnormal ingestion of nonfood items.
Air biting—repetitive snapping at the air for no obvious reason, exclusive of normal activities.
Abnormal suckling—preference by an animal to suckle on a human finger, body part, or clothes.
Anorexia—loss or lack of the appetite for food.
Chewing telephone cords—repetitive chewing with the molars on telephone cord or wires.
Cloth chewing—repetitive chewing with the molars on cloth material.
Coprophagy—ingestion of feces.
Excessive chewing—repetitive mastication when there are no longer any food materials in the mouth.
Fabric licking—repetitive and excessive licking of cloth material.
Foraging—searching for food.
Hyperphagia—eating a greater than optimal quantity of food.
Pica—ingestion of a variety of nonfood items and not limited to 1 particular substrate.
Psychogenic polydipsia—drinking of excessive amounts of water because of stress and not because of a medical condition.
Wood chewing—repetitive chewing with the molars on wood or wooden material.
Wool chewing—repetitive chewing with the molars on woolen material.
Wool sucking—repetitive sucking on woolen material.
Self-directed aggression—repetitive oral or body movements that are self-afflicted, cause the animal severe harm, are difficult or impossible to interrupt, and interfere with the animal's normal routine.
Self-mutilation—repetitive and excessive actions that result in ulcerations, bruises, or abrasions.
Tail chewing—repetitive and excessive chewing of the tail that may result in alopecia, abrasions, or ulcerations.
Grooming behavior—out of context, repetitive self-grooming movements that may injure the animal but do not cause severe harm, are difficult or impossible to interrupt, and interfere with the animal's normal routine.
Lick granuloma—licking in excess of normal grooming that results in areas of alopecia and abrasion.
Licking—licking in excess of normal grooming that does not result in areas of alopecia.
Excessive grooming—hair-pulling, licking, or chewing in excess of normal grooming that results in areas of alopecia.
Fears—behaviors resulting from the avoidance of triggers such as people, other animals, situations, or activities that the animal perceives as dangerous.
Fear of people
Fear of men—reluctance to be near or in the vicinity of men.
Fear of strangers—reluctance to be near or in the vicinity of an unfamiliar person.
Shyness—preferring to be alone and not interacting with the owners.
Fear of animals
Antisocial behavior—reluctance to be in the company of other members of the same species.
Fear of insects—reluctance to be near or in the vicinity of any type of insect.
Fear of other dogs—reluctance by a dog to be near or in the vicinity of other dogs. Fear of situations
Agoraphobia—fear of open spaces.
Resistance to handling—fear or reluctance to being picked up, held, or handled by a person.
House soiling—the release or deposition of feces or urine in an indoor location that is unacceptable to the owner.
Elimination—normal amounts of urine or feces deposited outside of areas the owner considers acceptable and variable amount of urine released during periods of excitement or while the animal is in any type of submissive posture.
Excitement urination—releasing small amounts of urine during periods of excitement.
Submissive urination—releasing small amounts of urine while in any type of submissive posture.
Urination and defecation—normal amounts of urine and feces deposited outside of areas the owner considers acceptable.
Defecation—normal amounts of feces deposited outside of areas the owner considers acceptable.
Urination—normal amounts of urine deposited outside of areas the owner considers acceptable.
Marking—depositing less than normal amounts of urine or stool in socially or olfactory important locations.
Urine marking—depositing less than normal amounts of urine on a vertical surface (spraying) or horizontal surface in socially or olfactory important locations, in response to social or olfactory stimuli, or both.
Miscellaneous—behaviors with criteria that do not fit well into any of the listed categories.
Cognitive dysfunction—an age-related syndrome caused by degeneration in the brain and a decline in higher brain functions, causing a group of signs such as disorientation, changes in interactions with owners, and changes in sleep-wake cycles and elimination patterns.
Depression—changes in appetite and sleep-wake cycle, often accompanied by social withdrawal.
Pseudocyesis—the condition of false pregnancy in which hormonal changes consistent with pregnancy result in similar behavioral changes.
Psychogenic salivation—salivating excessively because of stress and not because of a medical condition.
Hyperesthesia syndrome—a poorly understood syndrome also known as twitchy cat disease, rolling skin disease, and feline neurodermatitis. Behaviors seen include rippling of skin, rolling on the floor, and self-directed mutilation; these behaviors are usually accompanied by vocalization.
Phobias—an extreme fear response that is excessive and disproportionate to any real or potential threat or situation perceived as threatening.
Hysteria when approached—profound and extreme response to being approached by a person or another animal.
Noise phobia—profound and extreme response to noise other than thunder, resulting in escape, avoidance, and anxiety behaviors.
Panic attack—a profound response to a situation causing extreme anxiety.
Storm phobia—profound escape, avoidance, or anxiety behaviors in response to thunderstorms and their manifestations (rain, noise, lightning, darkness, wind, and changes in barometric pressure and ozone).
Sexual behavior—sexual activity that is either excessive or inappropriately directed.
Masturbation—self-manipulation of the genitals.
Mounting—thrusting the pelvis against animate or inanimate objects.
Unruly behavior—behaviors resulting from control and obedience problems, including medical causes for inadequate control.
Attention-seeking behavior—intrusive behavior initiated by the pet and continued until the pet has the owner's attention. The behavior may be active (such as jumping, vocalizing, pawing) or passive (such as staring, leaning against).
Destructive—destroys household property by digging, scratching, or chewing in presence or absence of owner.
Difficult to control—all situations in which the owner is unable to adequately control the dog, including jumping on people, running away, car chasing, and pulling while on lead.
Hyperactive—unable to relax, high heart and respiratory rates, and high temperature at rest with little increase when exercised.
Hyperexcitable—being overly stimulated or excited by situations, people, or objects.
Roaming—leaving the property for extended periods before returning, usually for sex, food, or hunting prey.
Running away—escaping from the property.
Scratching destructively—use of the claws on a surface the owner considers undesirable.
Vocalization behavior—excessive annoying sounds emitted by the animal in play, excitement, greeting, social facilitation, and attention-seeking behavior, but excluding vocalizations secondary to serious behavioral problems such as separation anxiety, aggression and cognitive dysfunction.

Contributor Notes

Dr. Bamberger's present address is Vet Behavior Consults, 1225 Hinging Post Rd, Ithaca, NY 14850.

The authors thank Dr. Robert Strawderman for advice on statistical analysis, Dr. Robert Oswald for advice on data entry and analysis, and Emma Williford and Doreen Turk for technical assistance.

Address correspondence to Dr. Bamberger.