Buprenorphine is a semi-synthetic, partial μ-opioid-receptor agonist drug that is commonly used for perioperative analgesia in rats.1,2 The drug has a half-life of 2.1 to 3 hours after IV bolus administration.1,3 The standard dosage of BH in rats (0.05 mg/kg, SC, q 6 to 8 hours)2,4–6 requires injections at a frequency associated with signs of stress, pica, reduced food intake, altered locomotor activity, and hyperalgesia.7–10 Results of previous studies11,12 to assess the effects of SRB in rats revealed that the treatment prolonged hypoalgesia (up to 72 hours), but had adverse effects of skin irritation (erythema and scabbing),11 reduced activity, and respiratory depression.13
Pica, the consumption of nonnutritive substrates (eg, bedding), is an adverse effect of buprenorphine administration that can result in reduced food intake and low weight gain, gastrointestinal obstruction, and even death in rats.9,14,15 Kaolin, a nonnutritive substance composed of acacia gum, has been safely used for repeatable measurement of pica behavior in rats.16–20
Commercially available HCB (1.8 mg/mL) is hypoalgesic for up to 24 hours in cats when administered at 0.24 mg/kg.21 However, when this same formulation was tested in healthy rats at 0.075, 0.15, and 0.30 mg/kg, only the highest dose was associated with hypoalgesic effects, which were detectable for a short duration (1 hour); all doses were associated with alterations in food intake and with self-injurious behavior.22 This finding, in a study22 performed by our group, led to speculation that an inability to express pica behavior as a manifestation of nausea might induce self-injurious behavior, because pica was prevented by housing the rats in wire-bottomed cages without bedding during the experiments.
The purpose of the study reported here was to evaluate self-injurious behavior, thermal sensitivity, food intake, fecal output, and pica after SC administration of BH, SRB, or HCB in healthy rats that did not undergo surgery or other potentially painful procedures. Kaolin was provided to the rats to allow pica behavior. We hypothesized that no significant self-injurious behavior would occur; that postinjection hind limb thermal withdrawal latencies would be higher than preinjection values for all buprenorphine formulations, with this increase lasting longer for SRB than for BH and HCB treatments; that postinjection food intake and fecal output would be reduced during the active (dark) 12-hour period, compared with that during the preinjection dark period; and that kaolin intake would be observed after all 3 buprenorphine treatments, with SRB associated with a more prolonged decrease in food intake and fecal output and a more prolonged increase in kaolin intake than the other formulations.
Materials and Methods
Animals
All experiments were approved by the University of Wisconsin Animal Care and Use Committee, and all rats were treated in compliance with Institute for Laboratory Animal Research guidelines.23 Eight adult male Sprague-Dawley rats (Rattus norvegicusa; mean ± SD body weight at the start of the study, 339 ± 11 g) were used. Rats were housed in pairs on corn cob bedding in an Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care–accredited facility with a 12-hour light (6:00 am to 6:00 pm) and 12-hour dark cycle (6:00 pm to 6:00 am). A commercial pelleted rat feedb and water were provided with access ad libitum.
Rats were allowed 7 days to acclimatize to the animal holding facility prior to testing and 48 hours to acclimatize to wire-bottomed cages before experiments. For 48 hours before and 24 hours after injection, rats were housed singly on wire-bottomed cages, with free access to the same commercial rat feedb and water. Rats were returned to their corn cob bedding for each drug washout period. The rats were certified (by the commercial supplier) free from ectromelia virus, Hantaan virus, murine pneumotropic virus, Kilham rat virus, lactic dehydrogenase elevating virus, lymphocytic virus, minute virus of mice, mouse adenoviruses type 1 and 2, mouse cytomegalovirus, mouse hepatitis virus, mouse parvovirus, mouse polyoma virus, mouse rotavirus, mouse thymic virus, murine norovirus, pneumonia virus of mice, rat minute virus, rat parvovirus, rat Theiler virus, respiratory enteric virus III, Sendai virus, sialodacryoadenitis virus, Theiler murine encephalomyelitis, Toolan H1 parvovirus, Bordetella bronchiseptica, Helicobacter spp, Mycoplasma pulmonis, Pasteurella multocida, dermatophytes, ectoparasites, and endoparasites. The rats were euthanized by CO2 inhalation at the end of the study in accordance with AVMA euthanasia guidelines.24
Study design
In a blinded, randomized,c crossover design, all rats received each of 4 treatments as a single SC injection (BH,d 0.05 mg/kg; SRB,e 1.2 mg/kg; HCB,f 0.30 mg/kg; or 5% dextrose solution,g 0.20 mL/kg). The investigator who performed all injections (MA) was blinded to treatment (opaque tape was used to obscure the injectate in each syringe). The volume of dextrose solution corresponded to the highest injectate volume, which was associated with the SRB treatment. Injections were given between 6:00 am and 7:00 am (immediately after the dark cycle). The dose for HCB was determined from a previous investigation by our group,22 in which only the selected dose (0.30 mg/kg) was associated with hypoalgesia. The doses for BH and SRB were determined from previously published studies.2,4–6,11,12 Rats underwent a 1-week washout period between treatments.
Self-injurious behavior
Self-injurious behavior (self-biting and cage-biting) was quantified before injection (time 0) and at 1, 4, 8, 12, and 24 hours after injection. Each observation period had a 2-minute duration. A descriptive scale adapted from previous studies10,25 and used in a recent study of HCB in rats22 was applied, where 0 = no cage-biting or self-biting; 1 = occasional cage-biting and no self-biting; 2 = frequent cage-biting, occasional to frequent self-biting with no visible evidence of skin trauma, or both; 3 = frequent self-biting with mild to moderate visible evidence of skin trauma (inflammation); and 4 = frequent self-biting with moderate to severe visible evidence of skin trauma (bleeding). Occasional cage-biting or self-biting was defined as occurring for ≤ 30 seconds (continuous or intermittent) during a given observation period. Frequent cage-biting or self-biting was defined as occurring for > 30 seconds during a single observation period.
Withdrawal responses to thermal stimuli
Hypoalgesia was assessed by measuring latency of hind limb withdrawal to a radiant heat stimulus26 applied with a commercial testing device.h Rats were allowed to acclimatize to the chambers for 15 minutes prior to testing. The intensity of the heat stimulus and rate of heating were kept constant throughout the study to establish a target withdrawal latency range of 7 to 9 seconds for preinjection measurements, with a maximum latency limit of 20 seconds to avoid thermal injury. An infrared heat stimulus was applied to a plantar surface, and the time to withdrawal of the paw in response to the stimulus was defined as the hind limb withdrawal latency. The latencies were measured before injection (time 0) and at 1, 4, 8, 12, and 24 hours after injection. Each rat was tested 3 times at each time point with ≥ 5 minutes between trials on alternating paws (≥ 10 minutes between trials on the same paw), and the mean latency was calculated. This pattern was chosen to minimize hyperalgesia secondary to repeated noxious stimuli.13,26,27 Hind limb withdrawal latencies were performed on both right and left hind limbs, and a mean value for both hind limbs was calculated, similar to previous studies using withdrawal latencies.22,28–32 Data were summarized as the percentage change from preinjection values (percentage change at time 0 = 0%), similar to previously published studies.13,22
Food intake, fecal output, and kaolin intake
Rats were provided free access to a premeasured amount of commercial rat feedb (250 ± 3 g) in a removable wire rack and a premeasured amount of kaolin pelletsi (32 ± 3 g) in a polystyrene Petri dish when they were housed singly in wire-bottomed cages 48 hours before each treatment. Paper liners were placed on cage bottoms to absorb urine and water so that spilled food and kaolin would not become saturated. The mass of food and kaolin remaining in the wire rack and Petri dish, respectively, and that spilled on the cage floor was measured every 12 hours from 48 hours before until 24 hours after injection. Spilled food and kaolin were separated prior to measurement. Fecal pellets were collected and weighed every 12 hours for the same time period. Measurements for individual rats were expressed as an index of body weight (mg/kg) because body weights varied over the course of the study (range, 315 to 402 g). Similar to the methods in a previous study,22 the mean food intake, kaolin intake, and fecal output for two 12-hour light and two 12-hour dark periods before injection (preinjection values) and one 12-hour light and one 12-hour dark period after injection (postinjection values) were calculated for each treatment to assess changes in food intake, kaolin intake (pica), and fecal output associated with circadian rhythms.33–37
Statistical analysis
All 8 rats received each treatment and had data collected at each predetermined time point. The Shapiro-Wilk test was used to determine whether data were normally distributed. Hind limb withdrawal latencies were compared between the left and right hind limbs with a Mann-Whitney rank sum test because of nonnormal distribution; the values were not significantly different, and data were subsequently combined, with mean values for the 2 limbs at each time point used for further analysis, similar to previous studies.28–32 Self-injurious behavior scores were expressed as median and IQR (25th to 75th percentile) and were analyzed with a Friedman repeated-measures ANOVA on ranks, with time (1, 4, 8, 12, and 24 hours) and treatment as independent factors. The percentage change in hind limb withdrawal latency from preinjection (time 0) values (normal distribution), food intake (mg/kg/12 hours; normal distribution), fecal output (mg/kg/12 hours; nonnormal distribution), and kaolin intake (mg/kg/12 hours; nonnormal distribution) measurements were expressed as mean ± SEM and were analyzed with a 2-way repeated-measures ANOVA, with time (1, 4, 8, 12, and 24 hours for hind limb withdrawal latency data; preinjection light or dark cycle or postinjection light or dark cycle for food intake, fecal output, and kaolin intake) and treatment as independent factors. The Student-Newman Keuls post hoc test was used to detect differences among the 4 treatments. Values of P ≤ 0.05 were considered significant. Statistical analysis was performed with commercially available software.j
Results
Self-injurious behavior
Self-injurious behavior (ie, a score ≥ 1/4) was absent in all preinjection (time 0) assessments. Median self-injurious behavior scores 1, 4, 8, and 12 hours after injection of SRB or HCB were significantly (P ≤ 0.001 for all comparisons) higher than preinjection scores for the same treatments. Self-injurious behavior scores were also significantly (P = 0.024) higher than preinjection scores 24 hours after SRB administration (Figure 1). No rats had to be removed from the study because of injury.
Differences were also observed among study treatments. Median self-injurious behavior scores for the SRB treatment were significantly (P ≤ 0.014 for all comparisons) higher than for the dextrose, BH, and HCB treatments at the 8- and 12-hour time points (Figure 1).
Hind limb withdrawal latencies
No significant (all P ≥ 0.75) differences in hind limb withdrawal latencies were found among treatments at preinjection (time 0) assessments. No significant (all P ≥ 0.14) differences in percentage change from preinjection values for hind limb withdrawal latencies were found among treatments from 1 to 12 hours after injection (Figure 2). However, at the 24-hour time point, the percentage change in hind limb withdrawal latencies was significantly (P = 0.014) higher for the SRB treatment than for the BH treatment. When compared within treatments over time, the percentage change from preinjection values in hind limb withdrawal latencies was significantly higher for the BH (all P ≤ 0.015) and HCB (all P ≤ 0.006) treatments 1 hour after injection, compared with all other time points for those treatments.
Food intake
There were no differences (all P ≥ 0.50) in food intake among treatments during the preinjection light period or the preinjection dark period (Figure 3). Preinjection food intake was significantly (all P < 0.001) higher during the dark period than during the light period for all treatments (with mean ± SD differences of 127 ± 25%).
Postinjection light-period food intake was significantly (all P ≤ 0.028) higher for the BH and HCB (all P = 0.002) treatments than for the dextrose treatment (with differences of 34% and 48%, respectively) or the SRB treatment (with differences of 37% and 51%, respectively). The postinjection light-period values for the BH and HCB treatments were also significantly (P ≤ 0.002 for both) greater than the preinjection light-period values for the same treatments (with differences of 68% and 58%, respectively; Figure 3).
Postinjection dark-period food intake was significantly (all P ≤ 0.004) lower for the BH, SRB, and HCB treatments than for the dextrose treatment (with differences of 21%, 24%, and 33%, respectively). Post-injection dark-period food intakes for the BH, SRB, and HCB treatments were also significantly (all P < 0.001) lower than the preinjection dark-period values for the same treatments (with differences of 28%, 35%, and 41%, respectively; Figure 3).
Fecal output
No significant (all P > 0.65) differences in fecal output were found among treatments at any time (Figure 4). Fecal output was significantly (all P ≤ 0.001) higher for all treatments during the dark period than during the light period for both preinjection (with differences of 77%, 81%, 47%, 57% for dextrose, BH, SRB, and HCB, respectively) and postinjection (62%, 128%, 200%, and 176% for dextrose, BH, SRB, and HCB, respectively) measurements.
Kaolin intake
Preinjection kaolin intake did not differ significantly (all P ≥ 0.08) among treatments or between light and dark periods (Figure 5). Postinjection light-period kaolin intake was significantly (P ≤ 0.021) higher for the SRB treatment than for the dextrose, BH, and HCB treatments (with differences of 3,445%, 134%, and 298%, respectively). Postinjection light-period kaolin intake was also significantly (P = 0.002) higher for the SRB treatment, compared with preinjection light-period intake for the same treatment (with a difference of 736%). Postinjection dark-period kaolin intake was significantly (P ≤ 0.008) higher for the SRB treatment than the dextrose, BH, and HCB treatments (with differences of 1,110%, 551%, and 254%, respectively).
Discussion
To our knowledge, the present study was the first to compare HCB with other commercially available buprenorphine formulations with regard to hypoalgesia, self-injurious behavior, food intake, fecal output, and pica behavior in rats. Each drug was administered SC, once, at a single predetermined dose (0.30, 1.2, and 0.05 mg/kg for HCB, SRB, and BH, respectively) in this crossover-design study. Although hypoalgesia (as determined by increased hind limb withdrawal latency in response to a thermal stimulus, relative to that prior to injection) was detected for a short period after administration of some buprenorphine formulations, self-injurious behavior in all buprenorphine-treated rats suggested that all of the tested formulations have the potential to cause unwanted effects when administered to healthy rats with no evidence of pain. Specifically, self-injurious behavior was not seen in rats that received the vehicle treatment (5% dextrose solution, 0.2 mL/kg, SC) but was observed in all rats during the first 8 hours after injection of BH, HCB, or SRB, with significantly increased median self-injurious behavior scores persisting for 12 hours in HCB-treated rats and for 24 hours in SRB-treated rats. The availability of kaolin pellets did not prevent self-injurious behavior, thus refuting 1 of the study hypotheses. Self-injurious behavior is observed in rats after treatment with neurostimulants such as amphetamines and morphine, with dopaminergic stimulation implicated as a potential cause.10,38–44 Self-injurious behavior associated with buprenorphine administration, however, appears to be rare or at least not well documented, with the exception of our previously described HCB study22 and a recent letter describing excessive licking and chewing after treatment with an extended-release formulation of buprenorphine,45 in which the authors equated the behavior with nausea. The low frequency of such reports is potentially attributable to most studies that involve the use of buprenorphine in rats being performed in animals undergoing surgery or other potentially painful procedures, unlike the rats in the present study.
Significant hypoalgesia was observed only with the BH and HCB treatments in the present study, and the results differed from preinjection (time 0) values only at the 1-hour time point. Our results are similar to previous investigations of BH and HCB in rats, in which hypoalgesia was detected for only 1 hour.13,22 In the present study, the percentage change from preinjection values was higher in SRB-treated rats than in BH-treated rats at 24 hours, but it was not significantly different from time 0 values or values at other time points for the SRB treatment. Similar to our findings, results of a study11 comparing effects of BH (at 0.2 mg/kg) and SRB (at 1.2 mg/kg) in rats revealed no significant difference between treatments in hind limb withdrawal latencies measured by means of thermal analgesiometry until 48 hours after injection. In that study,11 plasma concentrations of BH and SRB were similar until 8 hours after administration, and after that time point, only circulating SRB concentrations remained above the presumed therapeutic threshold of 1 ng/mL until 72 hours after injection.11 Overall, given that adequate plasma drug concentrations and hypoalgesia associated with SC administration of BH and SRB in rats2,4,5,11–13 and HCB in cats21 have been demonstrated previously, it is unclear why we did not detect significant changes from time 0 in hind limb withdrawal latencies with SRB and more prolonged hypoalgesic effects with BH and HCB.
Our use of a thermal stimulus to assess the hypoalgesic effects of buprenorphine in animals that did not undergo surgery likely influenced the results. Various types of noxious stimuli produce diverse responses, and it is possible that inflammatory conditions or the use of mechanical stimuli might have revealed evidence of hypoalgesia with all buprenorphine preparations. Furthermore, the observed responses to a thermal stimulus could potentially have been altered by opioid-induced effects, including pica, self-injurious behavior, hyperalgesia, sensitization, and tolerance, overriding any hypoalgesic effect. The first 2 factors were observed in the present study. The latter 3 effects, especially sensitization from repeated testing, were not measured and could not be ruled out. However, these influences should have been minimized by the study design. For example, hyperalgesia appears to be associated with repeated dosing, ultra-low doses, and low plasma drug concentrations, and occurs several hours to days after acute opioid treatment.8,46 In addition, testing intervals were chosen on the basis of information in previous studies26,27 to avoid sensitization, although this effect could not be completely ruled out. Finally, single dose administration, randomization of treatment order, and a 1-week washout period were chosen to minimize opioid tolerance. However, tolerance has been reported for up 10 days after a single postsurgical dose of buprenorphine > 0.05 mg/kg in rats.4 Therefore, this effect might have been further minimized by extending the washout period to 2 weeks, although this would have doubled the study duration.
In the present study, we chose to separate food intake, fecal output, and kaolin intake data into light and dark periods to account for circadian rhythms.33–37 In previous studies,22,34,35 food intake in rats was highest during the 12-hour dark period when they are most active. Our study confirmed that in untreated rats, food intake was higher during the dark period than during the light period. After buprenorphine administration, this relationship was altered, with food intake becoming higher during the light period (BH and HCB), and lower during the dark period (BH, SRB, and HCB) compared with the dextrose treatment and with preinjection values. These findings suggested that rats became more active after buprenorphine administration and that caloric requirements were met by consuming more food during the light period. Given that postinjection dark-period food intake for rats with the SRB treatment was lower than that for the dextrose treatment and preinjection dark-period food intake for the SRB treatment (similar to BH and HCB treatments), the authors speculate that the lack of a significant increase in postinjection light-period food intake for this treatment (unlike BH and HCB treatments) might have been attributable to the high kaolin intake observed with that treatment or to small sample size.
Preinjection and postinjection fecal output was significantly higher during the dark period than during the light period when all treatments were combined for analysis. In a previous study22 by the authors investigating HCB in rats, fecal output was altered during the first 12 hours (light period) after treatment, which the authors attributed to the inhibitory effects of buprenorphine on gastrointestinal motility, an effect of buprenorphine described in other studies.1,47 The lack of a significant alteration in the relationship between light-period and dark-period fecal output after treatment in the present study, despite altered food intake patterns, may have resulted from kaolin intake in the present study. Kaolin passes easily through the gastrointestinal tract,16–20 and, indeed, many large, white fecal pellets were collected throughout the experiments (separation of lighter [primarily kaolin-containing] vs darker, normal-appearing fecal pellets was not performed).
Preinjection kaolin intake was similar among all treatments during the light and dark periods. Postinjection kaolin intake was higher for the SRB treatment than for all other treatments during both light and dark periods. Postinjection kaolin intake during the light period for the SRB treatment was also higher than preinjection light period values for the same treatment. On the basis of these findings, we speculate that SRB may have greater nausea-inducing effects in healthy rats with no evidence of pain than the other tested buprenorphine formulations.
Overall, hypoalgesic effects in response to a thermal noxious stimulus were observed for only 1 hour after administration of 2 of the 3 buprenorphine formulations tested, and self-injurious behavior, pica, or both may have overridden hypoalgesic effects in these healthy laboratory rats. Opioid-induced hyperalgesia, sensitization, and tolerance could also not be completely ruled out. Further studies comparing the effects of BH, SRB, and HCB in rats undergoing surgery or exposed to noxious stimuli are indicated to elucidate whether these formulations have clinical benefits in this species.
Acknowledgments
Funded by the Companion Animal Foundation, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Wisconsin. The authors declare that there were no conflicts of interest.
ABBREVIATIONS
BH | Buprenorphine hydrochloride |
HCB | High-concentration buprenorphine |
IQR | Interquartile (25th to 75th percentile) range |
SRB | Sustained-release buprenorphine |
Footnotes
Harlan Laboratories, Indianapolis, Ind.
Teklad 6% fat mouse/rat diet, Envigo, Madison, Wis.
Randomizer.org. Research randomizer. Available at: www.randomizer.org. Accessed Apr 19, 2017.
Buprenex, 0.3 mg/mL, Reckitt Benckiser, Hull, England.
Buprenorphine SR Lab, 1 mg/mL, ZooPharm, Windsor, Colo.
Simbadol, 1.8 mg/mL, Zoetis Inc, Kalamazoo, Mich.
Baxter Healthcare, Deerfield, Ill.
Ugo Basile, Varese, Italy.
Research Diets, New Brunswick, NJ.
SigmaPlot version 12.0, Systat Software, San Jose, Calif.
References
1. Cowan A, Lewis JW, MacFarlane IR. Agonist and antagonist properties of buprenorphine, a new antinociceptive agent. Br J Pharmacol 1977;60:537–545.
2. Flecknell P. Analgesia and post-operative care. In: Laboratory animal anaesthesia. 3rd ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 2009;139–179.
3. Ohtani M, Kotaki H, Nishitateno K, et al. Kinetics of respiratory depression in rats induced by buprenorphine and its metabolite, norbuprenorphine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1997;281:428–433.
4. Curtin LI, Grakowsky JA, Suarez M, et al. Evaluation of buprenorphine in a postoperative pain model in rats. Comp Med 2009;59:60–71.
5. Schaap MWH, Uilenreef JJ, Mitsogiannis MD, et al. Optimizing the dosing interval of buprenorphine in rats. Lab Anim 2012;46:287–292.
6. Thompson AC, Kristal MB, Abdullah S, et al. Analgesic efficacy of orally administered buprenorphine in rats. Comp Med 2004;54:293–300.
7. Bourque SL, Adams MA, Nakatsu K, et al. Comparison of buprenorphine and meloxicam for postsurgical analgesia in rats: effects on body weight, locomotor activity, and hemodynamic parameters. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2010;49:617–622.
8. Cooper DM, Hoffman W, Wheat N, et al. Duration of effects on clinical parameters and referred hyperalgesia in rats after abdominal surgery and multiple doses of analgesia. Comp Med 2005;55:344–353.
9. Raffa RB, Porreca F, Cowan A, et al. Morphine receptor dissociation constant and the stimulus-effect relation for inhibition of gastrointestinal transit in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol 1982;79:11–16.
10. Sharma R, Manchanda SK, Nayar U. Role of opioid receptors in self-aggression in rats. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 1991;35:165–169.
11. Foley PL, Liang H, Crichlow AR. Evaluation of a sustained-release formulation of buprenorphine for analgesia in rats. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2011;50:198–204.
12. Chum HH, Jampachairsri K, McKeon GP, et al. Antinociceptive effects of sustained-release buprenorphine in a model of incisional pain in rats (Rattus norvegicus). J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2014;53:193–197.
13. Johnson RA. Voluntary running-wheel activity, arterial blood gases, and thermal antinociception in rats after 3 buprenorphine formulations. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2016;55:306–311.
14. Clark JA Jr, Myers PH, Goelz MF, et al. Pica behavior associated with buprenorphine administration in the rat. Lab Anim Sci 1997;47:300–303.
15. Jacobson C. Adverse effects on growth rates in rats caused by buprenorphine administration. Lab Anim 2000;34:202–206.
16. De Jonghe BC, Lawler MP, Horn CC, et al. Pica as an adaptive response: kaolin consumption helps rats recover from chemotherapy-induced illness. Physiol Behav 2009;97:87–90.
17. Goineau S, Castagne V. Comparison of three preclinical models for nausea and vomiting assessment. J Pharmacol Toxicol Methods 2016;82:45–53.
18. Shi J. Evaluating the various phases of cisplatin-induced emesis in rats. Oncol Lett 2014;8:2017–2022.
19. Takeda N, Hasegawa S, Masahiro M, et al. Pica in rats is analogous to emesis: an animal model in emesis research. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1993;45:817–821.
20. Yamamoto K, Nakai M, Nohara K, et al. The anti-cancer drug-induced pica in rats is related to their clinical emetogenic potential. Eur J Pharmacol 2007;554:34–39.
21. US FDA. Simbadol buprenorphine injection. Freedom of information summary. Original new animal drug application. NADA 141–434. Sponsored by Abbott Laboratories. Available at: www.fda.gov/downloads/AnimalVeterinary/Products/ApprovedAnimalDrugProducts/FOIADrugSummaries/UCM408852.pdf. Accessed Mar 3, 2017.
22. Allen M, Nietlisbach N, Johnson RA. Evaluation of self-injurious behavior, food intake, fecal output, and thermal withdrawal latencies after injection a high-concentration buprenorphine formulation in rats (Rattus norvegicus). Am J Vet Res 2018;79:154–162.
23. Institute for Laboratory Animal Research. Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals. 8th ed. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2011.
24. AVMA. AVMA guidelines for the euthanasia of animals: 2013 edition. Available at: www.avma.org/KB/Policies/Documents/euthanasia.pdf. Accessed Mar 3, 2017.
25. Creese I, Iversen S. Blockage of amphetamine induced motor stimulation and stereotypy in the adult rat following neonatal treatment with 6-hydroxydopamine. Brain Res 1973;55:369–382.
26. Hargreaves K, Dubner R, Brown F, et al. A new and sensitive method for measuring thermal nociception in cutaneous hyperalgesia. Pain 1988;32:77–88.
27. Le Bars D, Gozariu M, Cadden SW. Animal models of nociception. Pharmacol Rev 2001;53:597–652.
28. Meng X, Zhang Y, Lao L, et al. Spinal interleukin-17 promotes thermal hyperalgesia and NMDA NR1 phosphorylation in an inflammatory pain rat model. Pain 2013;154:294–305.
29. Zhang Y, Li A, Lao L, et al. Rostral ventromedial μ, but not κ, opioid receptors are involved in electroacupuncture anti-hyperalgesia in an inflammatory pain rat model. Brain Res 2011;1395:38–45.
30. Cheppudira BP. Characterization of hind paw licking and lifting to noxious radiant heat in the rat with and without chronic inflammation. J Neurosci Methods 2006;155:122–125.
31. Buerkle H, Yaksh T. Comparison of the spinal actions of the μ-opioid remifentanil with alfentanil and morphine in the rat. Anesthesiology 1996;84:94–102.
32. Endo D, Ikeda T, Ishida Y, et al. Effect of intrathecal administration of hemokinin-1 on the withdrawal response to noxious stimulation of the rat hind paw. Neurosci Lett 2006;392:114–117.
33. Aschoff J. Exogenous and endogenous components in circadian rhythms. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 1960;25:11–28.
34. Siegel PS, Stuckey HL. The diurnal course of water and food intake in the normal mature rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1947;40:365–370.
35. Siegel PS. Food intake in the rat in relation to the dark-light cycle. Physiol Psychol 1961;54:294–301.
36. Terman M, Terman JS. Control of the rat's circadian self-stimulation rhythm by light-dark cycles. Physiol Behav 1975;14:781–789.
37. Zucker I. Light-dark rhythms in rat eating and drinking behavior. Physiol Behav 1971;6:115–126.
38. Fog R. Behavioural effects in rats of morphine and amphetamine and a combination of the two drugs. Psychopharmacologia 1970;16:305–312.
39. Inbal R, Devor M, Tuchendler O, et al. Autonomy following nerve injury: genetic factors in the development of chronic pain. Pain 1980;9:327–337.
40. Mogilnicka E, Braestrup C. Noradrenergic influence on the stereotyped behaviour induced by amphetamine, phenethylamine and apomorphine. J Pharm Pharmacol 1976;28:253–255.
41. Mueller K, Saboda S, Palmour R, et al. Self-injurious behavior produced in rats by daily caffeine and continuous amphetamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1982;17:613–617.
42. Pollock J, Kornetsky C. Evidence for the role of dopamine D1 receptors in morphine induced stereotypic behavior. Neurosci Lett 1989;102:291–296.
43. Knapp CM, Jha SH, Kornetsky C. Increased sensitization to morphine-induced oral stereotypy in aged rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2004;79:491–497.
44. Mori T, Ito S, Kita T, et al. Effects of μ-, δ- and κ-opioid receptor agonists on methamphetamine-induced self-injurious behavior in mice. Eur J Pharmacol 2006;532:81–87.
45. Sarabia-Estrada R, Cowan A, Tyler BM, et al. Association of nausea with buprenorphine analgesia for rats. Lab Anim (NY) 2017;46:242–244.
46. Wala EP, Holtman JR. Buprenorphine-induced hyperalgesia in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol 2011;651:89–95.
47. Cowan A. Buprenorphine and gastrointestinal transit in rats: effect of naloxone on the biphasic dose-response curve. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 1992;19:47–49.